Rodney Brooks (iRobot Co-Founder) – MIT AI Lab Lecture: Artificial Humanity (1997)
I see that it seems to me that most scientists engage in a certain cultural constructivism. All researchers base their work on some, usually unstated, dogmatic beliefs. And this gets a lot of people upset because a lot of people believe that they’re in search of truth and there is one truth. But I think that when you push people, who speak that way, maybe those beliefs are in a scientific method, which has a certain religious aura about it when a lot of people talk about it. Or whether they believe, as I do, that humans ultimately have mechanistic explanations, when it gets down to it, we can’t explain it beyond that in any logical way. We base our scientific lives on some set of assumptions. And I’ve observed that challenges to these implicit beliefs are often met with hostility.
– Brooks @ 18:36
Chapters
Abstract
Artificial humanity—a paradoxical combination of words that’s increasingly at the forefront of scientific and philosophical discussions. Rodney Brooks, in his enlightening talk titled “Artificial Humanity,” wades through the intricate layers of robotics and human perception. Brooks muses over whether robots can ever reach a level of humanity that we can recognize and empathize with. As we grapple with these questions, it’s imperative to remember that humanity’s evolution and perceptions have been molded by historical milestones, like Galileo’s heliocentric universe and the mechanisms of DNA.
Human history is replete with shifts in our understanding. From Galileo’s realization that Earth wasn’t the universe’s nucleus to Darwin’s revelations of common ancestry, every discovery has elicited both awe and unease. Our discomfort is rooted in a fear of losing our perceived ‘specialness.’ Turing’s propositions about mechanizing thought and the growth in biotechnology continue this legacy. A case in point is the humanoid robot “Cog.” While it mirrors human experiences to an extent, Brooks warns against the pitfall of “cargo cult science”, where imitation occurs without deeper comprehension.
While humanoid robots, especially from Japan, are gaining traction, their capacity for advanced intelligence or emotions remains constrained. As of the late ’90s, the drive was primarily towards achieving physical resemblances, evident from projects like Honda’s P2 and P3. The discussion extends beyond mere mechanics. Brooks, identifying as an atheist, contends with a mechanistic worldview that envisions humans and emotions as explainable down to minuscule molecular machines. This perspective, while scientifically cogent, contrasts with the emotional and humanistic aspects that govern daily life.
Peeling back another layer reveals that scientists often operate within cultural constructivism. Their endeavors frequently spring from implicit dogmatic beliefs that when challenged can elicit backlash. Brooks, through personal anecdotes and acute observations, brings forth the intricate balance between our natural predisposition to empathize with machines and the genuine depth of emotions these machines can achieve.
The concept of robot emotion is multifaceted. Brooks presents three distinct models for embedding emotions in robots: surface-level, subsurface, and emergent. While the first two are relatively straightforward, the last is elusive but captivating. Emotions serve pivotal roles in shaping robot behavior, especially when they’re bombarded with competing tasks. Yet, the perception of these robotic behaviors is also heavily colored by the observer. Actions that might be simple programmatic responses can be perceived as genuine emotional reactions.
As we delve deeper into robots’ emotional landscape, we confront questions of souls and self-preservation. Brooks provocatively suggests that if one believes humans possess souls due to their ability to experience emotions, this belief might logically extend to machines. From an evolutionary standpoint, Darwinian urgency for self-preservation rooted in biological constraints contrasts starkly with the “von Neumann evolution” of robots. If future robots require nurturing to develop, human interactions might shape their behavior, forging a new lineage of mechanical entities embodying unique cognitive content.
Q&A
During the Q&A, Brooks discusses the potential for paradigm shifts in scientific understanding and examines the nature of consciousness. Brooks challenges current definitions and mechanistic explanations of consciousness, considering the possibility of machines having subjective experiences. He rejects the homunculus argument and explores the idea of programming AI with drives and emergent behavior. The talk delves into the complex issue of emotion in AI, differentiating between descriptor and experiential emotion. Brooks also addresses the potential benefits and challenges of humanoid robot forms, debating whether they can truly have emotions or experiences.
Brooks’ insights on robotics and human psychology outline the distinctions and connections between the two fields. He emphasizes that building humanoid robots won’t unveil human consciousness but can challenge and refine psychological theories. He discusses the moral complexity of engaging with robots and the notion of determinism vs. non-determinism in biological processes. Brooks rejects the idea of downloading human consciousness, focusing instead on a robot’s physical experiences. He explores the moral implications of creating humanoid robots and the ensuing questions of acknowledgment and respect for sentient beings.
Notes by: Systemic01